Announcement

Collapse
No announcement yet.

Movement Systems: BBS Special Call for Commentators

Collapse
This topic is closed.
X
X
 
  • Filter
  • Time
  • Show
Clear All
new posts

  • Movement Systems: BBS Special Call for Commentators


    Below are the abstracts of 8 forthcoming target articles for a special
    issue on Movement Systems that will appear in Behavioral and Brain
    Sciences (BBS), an international, interdisciplinary journal that
    provides Open Peer Commentary on important and controversial current
    research in the biobehavioral and cognitive sciences. This will be the
    first in a new series called "Controversies in Neuroscience," done in
    collaboration with Paul Cordo and the RS Dow Neurological Science
    Institute.

    Commentators must be current BBS Associates or nominated by a current
    BBS Associate. To be considered as a commentator on any of these
    articles, to suggest other appropriate commentators, or for information
    about how to become a BBS Associate, please send email to:

    harnad@clarity.princeton.edu or harnad@pucc.bitnet or write to:
    BBS, 20 Nassau Street, #240, Princeton NJ 08542 [tel: 609-921-7771]

    Please specify which article or articles you would like to comment on.
    (Commentators will be allotted 1000 words to comment on one of the
    articles, 750 words more to comment on two of them, 500 more for three
    and then 250 more for each additional one, for a maximum of 3500 words
    to comment on all eight target articles.)

    To help us put together a balanced list of commentators, please give
    some indication of the aspects of the topic on which you would bring
    your areas of expertise to bear if you were selected as a commentator.
    In the next week or so, electronic drafts of the full text of each
    article will be available for inspection by anonymous ftp according to
    the instructions that follow after the abstracts. These drafts are for
    inspection only; please do not prepare a commentary until you are
    formally invited to do so.

    __________________________________________________ __________________

    1. Alexander GE, MR De Long, & MD Crutcher: DO CORTICAL AND BASAL
    GANGLIONIC MOTOR AREAS USE "MOTOR PROGRAMS" TO CONTROL MOVEMENT?
    bbs.alexander

    2. Bizzi E, N Hogan, FA Mussa-Ivaldi & S Giszter: DOES THE NERVOUS
    SYSTEM USE EQUILIBRIUM-POINT CONTROL TO GUIDE SINGLE AND MULTIPLE
    JOINT MOVEMENTS? bbs.bizzi

    3. Bloedel JR: DOES THE ONE-STRUCTURE/ONE-FUNCTION RULE APPLY TO THE
    CEREBELLUM? bbs.bloedel

    4. Fetz EH: ARE MOVEMENT PARAMETERS RECOGNIZABLY CODED IN SINGLE
    NEURON ACTIVITY? bbs.fetz

    5. Gandevia SC & D Burke: DOES THE NERVOUS SYSTEM DEPEND ON
    KINESTHETIC INFORMATION TO CONTROL NATURAL LIMB MOVEMENTS?
    bbs.gandevia

    6. McCrea DA: CAN SENSE BE MADE OF SPINAL INTERNEURON CIRCUITS?
    bbs.mccrea

    7. Robinson DA: IMPLICATIONS OF NEURAL NETWORKS FOR HOW WE THINK
    ABOUT BRAIN FUNCTION bbs.robinson

    8. Stein JF: POSTERIOR PARIETAL CORTEX AND EGOCENTRIC SPACE
    bbs.stein

    ----------------------------------------------------------------
    1. DO CORTICAL AND BASAL GANGLIONIC MOTOR AREAS
    USE "MOTOR PROGRAMS" TO CONTROL MOVEMENT?

    Garrett E. Alexander, Mahlon R. De Long, and Michael D. Crutcher
    Department of Neurology
    Emory University School of Medicine
    Atlanta, GA 30322
    gea@vax3200.neuro.emory.edu

    KEYWORDS: basal ganglia, cortex, motor system, motor program, motor
    control, parallel processing, connectionism, neural network

    ABSTRACT: Prevailing engineering-inspired theories of motor control
    based on sequential/algorithmic or motor programming models are
    difficult to reconcile with what is known about the anatomy and
    physiology of the motor areas. This is partly because of certain
    problems with the theories themselves and partly because of features of
    the cortical and basal ganglionic motor circuits that seem ill-suited
    for most engineering analyses of motor control. Recent developments in
    computational neuroscience offer more realistic connectionist models of
    motor processing. The distributed, highly parallel, and nonalgorithmic
    processes in these models are inherently self-organizing and hence more
    plausible biologically than their more traditional algorithmic or
    motor-programming counterparts. The newer models also have the
    potential to explain some of the unique features of natural,
    brain-based motor behavior and to avoid some of the computational
    dilemmas asscociated with engineering approaches.

    -------------------------------------------------------------------
    2. DOES THE NERVOUS SYSTEM USE EQUILIBRIUM-POINT CONTROL
    TO GUIDE SINGLE AND MULTIPLE JOINT MOVEMENTS?

    E. Bizzi, N. Hogan, F.A. Mussa-Ivaldi and S. Giszter
    Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences and
    Department of Mechanical Engineering
    Massachusetts Institute of Technology
    Cambridge, MA 02139
    emilio@wheaties.ai.mit.edu

    KEYWORDS: spinal cord, force field, equilibrium point,
    microstimulation, multi-joint coordination, contact tasks, robotics,
    inverse dynamics, motor control.

    ABSTRACT: The hypothesis that the central nervous system (CNS)
    generates movement as a shift of the limb's equilibrium posture has
    been corroborated experimentally in single- and multi-joint motions.
    Posture may be controlled through the choice of muscle length tension
    curves that set agonist-antagonist torque-angle curves determining an
    equilibrium position for the limb and the stiffness about the joints.
    Arm trajectories seem to be generated through a control signal defining
    a series of equilibrium postures.

    The equilibrium-point hypothesis drastically simplifies the requisite
    computations for multijoint movements and mechanical interactions with
    complex dynamic objects in the environment. Because the neuromuscular
    system is springlike, the instantaneous difference between the arm's
    actual position and the equilibrium position specified by the neural
    activity can generate the requisite torques, avoiding the complex
    "inverse dynamic" problem of computing the torques at the joints.

    The hypothesis provides a simple unified description of posture and
    movement as well as performance on contact control tasks, in which the
    limb must exert force stably and do work on objects in the environment.
    The latter is a surprisingly difficult problem, as robotic experience
    has shown.

    The prior evidence for the hypothesis came mainly from psychophysical
    and behavioral experiments. Our recent work has shown that
    microstimulation of the spinal cord's premotoneuronal network produces
    leg movements to various positions in the frog's motor space. The
    hypothesis can now be investigated in the neurophysiological machinery
    of the spinal cord.

    --------------------------------------------------------------------
    3. DOES THE ONE-STRUCTURE/ONE-FUNCTION RULE APPLY TO THE CEREBELLUM?

    James R. Bloedel
    Division of Neurobiology
    Barrow Neurological Institute
    Phoenix, AZ

    KEYWORDS: cerebellum; Purkinje cells; mossy fibres; movement;
    proprioception; body image; kinesthesis; robotics; posture.

    ABSTRACT: The premise explored in this target article is that the
    function of the cerebellum can be best understood in terms of the
    operation it performs across its structurally homogeneous subdivisions.
    The functional heterogeneity sometimes ascribed to these different
    regions reflects the many functions of the central targets receiving
    the outputs of different cerebellar regions. Recent studies by
    ourselves and others suggest that the functional unit of the cerebellum
    is its sagittal zone. It is hypothesized that the climbing fiber system
    produces a short-lasting modification in the gain of Purkinje cell
    responses to its other principle afferent input, the mossy
    fiber-granule cell-parallel fiber system. Because the climbing fiber
    inputs to sagittally aligned Purkinje cells can be activated under
    functionally specific conditions, they could select populations of
    Purkinje neurons that were most highly modulated by the distributed
    mossy fiber inputs responding to the same conditions. These operations
    may be critical for the on-line integration of inputs representing
    external target space with features of intended movement,
    proprioceptive and kinesthetic cues, and body image.

    -----------------------------------------------------------------
    4. ARE MOVEMENT PARAMETERS RECOGNIZABLY CODED
    IN SINGLE NEURON ACTIVITY?

    Eberhard E. Fetz
    Regional Primate Research Center
    University of Washington
    Seattle, WA 98195
    fetz@locke.hs.washington.edu

    KEYWORDS: neural coding; representation; neural networks;
    cross-correlation; movement parameters; parallel distributed processing

    ABSTRACT: To investigate neural mechanisms of movement, physiologists
    have analyzed the activity of task-related neurons in behaving animals.
    The relative onset latencies of neural activity have been scrutinized
    for evidence of a functional hierarchy of sequentially recruited
    centers, but activity appears to change largely in parallel. Neurons
    whose activity covaries with movement parameters have been sought for
    evidence of explicit coding of parameters such as active force, limb
    displacement and behavioral set. Neurons with recognizable relations to
    the task are typically selected from a larger population, ignoring
    unmodulated cells as well as cells whose activity is not related to the
    task in a simple, easily recognized way. Selective interpretations are
    also used to support the notion that different motor regions perform
    different motor functions; again, current evidence suggests that units
    with similar properties are widely distributed over different regions.

    These coding issues are re-examined for premotoneuronal (PreM) cells,
    whose correlational links with motoneurons are revealed by
    spike-triggered averages. PreM cells are recruited over long times
    relative to their target muscles. They show diverse response patterns
    relative to the muscle force they produce; functionally disparate PreM
    cells such as afferent fibers and descending corticomotoneuronal and
    rubromotoneuronal cells can exhibit similar patterns. Neural mechanisms
    have been further elucidated by neural network simulations of
    sensorimotor behavior; the pre-output hidden units typically show
    diverse responses relative to their targets. Thus, studies in which
    both the activity and the connectivity of the same units is known
    reveal that units with many kinds of relations to the task, simple and
    complex, contribute significantly to the output. This suggests that the
    search for explicit coding may be diverting us from understanding more
    distributed neural mechanisms that are more complex and less directly
    related to explicit movement paremeters.

    ------------------------------------------------------------------
    5. DOES THE NERVOUS SYSTEM DEPEND ON KINESTHETIC
    INFORMATION TO CONTROL NATURAL LIMB MOVEMENTS?

    S.C. Gandevia and David Burke
    Department of Clinical Neurophysiology
    Institute of Neurological Sciences
    The Prince Henry Hospital
    P.O. Box 233
    Matraville, N.S.W. 2036
    Sydney, Australia

    KEYWORDS: kinesthesia, motor control, muscle, joint and cutaneous
    afferents, motor commands, deafferentation

    ABSTRACT: This target article draws together two groups of
    experimental studies on the control of human movement through
    peripheral feedback and centrally generated signals of motor command.
    First, during natural movement, feedback from muscle, joint and
    cutaneous afferents changes; in human subjects these changes have
    reflexive and kinesthetic consequences. Recent psychophysical and
    microneurographic evidence suggests that joint and even cutaneous
    afferents may have a proprioceptive role. Second, the role of centrally
    generated motor commands in the control of normal movements and
    movements following acute and chronic of deafferentation is reviewed.
    There is increasing evidence that subjects can perceive their motor
    commands under various conditions, but this is inadequate for normal
    movement; deficits in motor performance arise when the reliance on
    proprioceptive feedback is abolished, either experimentally or because
    of pathology. During natural movement, the CNS appears to have access
    to functionally useful input from a range of receptors as well as from
    internally generated command signals. Remaining unanswered questions
    suggest a number of avenues for further research.

    ------------------------------------------------------------------
    6. CAN SENSE BE MADE OF SPINAL INTERNEURON CIRCUITS?

    David A. McCrea
    The Department of Physiology
    Faculty of Medicine
    University of Manitoba
    770 Bannatyne Avenue
    Winnipeg, Manitoba, Canada R3E OW3
    dave@scrc.umanitoba.ca

    KEYWORDS: interneuron, motor control, reflexes, spinal cord, flexion,
    muscle synery, presynaptic inhibition.

    ABSTRACT: It is increasingly clear that spinal reflex systems cannot be
    described in terms of simple and constant reflex actions. The extensive
    convergence of segmental and descending systems onto spinal
    interneurons suggests that spinal interneurons are not relay systems
    but rather form a crucial component in determining which muscles are
    activated during voluntary and reflex movements. The notion that
    descending systems simply modulate the gain of spinal interneuronal
    pathways has been tempered by the observation that spinal interneurons
    gate and distribute descending control to specific motoneurons. Spinal
    systems are complex, but current approaches will continue to provide
    insight into motor systems. During movement, several neural mechanisms
    act to reduce the functional complexity of motor systems by inhibiting
    some of the parallel reflex pathways available to segmental afferents
    and descending systems. The flexion reflex system is discussed as an
    example of the flexibility of spinal interneuron systems and as useful
    construct. Examples are provided of the kinds of experiments that can
    be developed using current approaches to spinal interneuronal systems.

    --------------------------------------------------------------------
    7. IMPLICATIONS OF NEURAL NETWORKS
    FOR HOW WE THINK ABOUT BRAIN FUNCTION

    David A. Robinson
    Ophthalmology, Biomedical Engineering, and Neuroscience
    The Johns Hopkins University, School of Medicine
    Room 355 Woods Res. Bldg.
    The Wilmer Institute
    Baltimore, MD 21205

    KEYWORDS: Neural networks, signal processing, oculomotor system,
    vestibulo-ocular reflex, pursuit eye movements, saccadic eye movements,
    coordinate transformations

    ABSTRACT: Engineers use neural networks to control systems too complex
    for conventional engineering analysis. To examine hidden unit behavior
    would defeat the purpose of this approach, because individual units
    would be largely uninterpretable. Yet neurophysiologists spend their
    careers doing just that! Hidden units contain bits and pieces of
    signals that yield only arcane hints of network function and no
    information about how the units process signals. Most of the literature
    on single-unit recordings attests to this grim fact. On the other hand,
    knowing system function and describing it with elegant mathematics
    tells one very little about what to expect of interneuron behavior.
    Examples of simple networks based on neurophysiology are taken from the
    oculomotor literature to suggest how single-unit interpretability might
    degrade with increasing task complexity. Trying to explain how any real
    neural network works on a cell-by-cell, reductionist basis is futile;
    we may have to be content with understanding the brain at higher levels
    of organization.

    -------------------------------------------------------------------
    8. POSTERIOR PARIETAL CORTEX AND EGOCENTRIC SPACE

    J.F. Stein
    University Laboratory of Physiology
    University of Oxford
    Oxford, England OX1 3PT
    stein@vax.oxford.ac.uk

    KEYWORDS: posterior parietal cortex; egocentric space; space
    perception; attention; coordinate transformations; distributed
    systems; neural networks.

    ABSTRACT: The posterior parietal cortex (PPC) is the most likely site
    where egocentric spatial relationships are represented in the brain.
    PPC cells receive visual, auditory, somaesthetic and vestibular sensory
    inputs, oculomotor, head, limb and body motor signals, and strong
    motivational projections from the limbic system. Their discharge
    increases not only when an animal moves towards a sensory target, but
    also when it directs its attention to it. PPC lesions have the opposite
    effect: sensory inattention and neglect. PPC does not seem to contain a
    "map" of the location of objects in space but a distributed neural
    network for transforming one set of sensory vectors into other sensory
    reference frames or into various motor coordinate systems. Which set of
    transformation rules is used probably depends on attention, which
    selectively enhances the synapses needed for a making particular
    sensory comparison or aiming a particular movement.

    --------------------------------------------------------------

    To help you decide whether you would be an appropriate commentator for
    any of these articles, a (nonfinal) draft of each will soon be
    retrievable by anonymous ftp from princeton.edu according to the
    instructions below (filenames will be of the form bbs.alexander, based
    on the name of the first author). Please do not prepare a commentary on
    this draft. Just let us know, after having inspected it, what relevant
    expertise you feel you would bring to bear on what aspect of the
    article.

    ---------------------------------------------------------------
    To retrieve a file by ftp from a Unix/Internet site,
    type either:
    ftp princeton.edu
    or
    ftp 128.112.128.1
    When you are asked for your login, type:
    anonymous
    For your password, type your real name.
    then change directories with:
    cd pub/harnad
    To show the available files, type:
    ls
    Next, retrieve the file you want with (for example):
    get bbs.alexander
    When you have the file(s) you want, type:
    quit

    JANET users can use the Internet file transfer utility at JANET node
    UK.AC.FT-RELAY to get BBS files. Use standard file transfer, setting
    the site to be UK.AC.FT-RELAY, the userid as anonymous@edu.princeton,
    the password as your own userid, and the remote filename to be the
    filename according to Unix conventions (e.g. pub/harnad/bbs.article).
    Lower case should be used where indicated, using quotes if necessary to
    avoid automatic translation into upper case.

    ---------------------------------------------------------------
    The above cannot be done form Bitnet directly, but there
    is a fileserver called bitftp@pucc.bitnet that will do
    it for you. Send it the one line message
    help
    for instructions (which will be similar to the above,
    but will be in the form of a series of lines in an
    email message that bitftp will then execute for you).
Working...
X